Skip navigation

Schnittker Incarceration Effects Pyschiatric Disorders 2011

Download original document:
Brief thumbnail
This text is machine-read, and may contain errors. Check the original document to verify accuracy.
Out and Down:
The Effects of Incarceration on Psychiatric Disorders and Disability
Jason Schnittker
University of Pennsylvania
Department of Sociology
3718 Locust Walk
Philadelphia, PA 19104-6299
jschnitt@ssc.upenn.edu
Michael Massoglia
The Pennsylvania State University
Christopher Uggen
University of Minnesota
February 3, 2011

Abstract
Although psychiatric disorders are common among current and former inmates, a putative
causal relationship is contaminated by assorted influences, including childhood disadvantage, the
early onset of most disorders, and the criminalization of substance use, which is itself comorbid
with a variety of other subsequent psychiatric disorders. Using the National Comorbidity Survey
Replication, this study examines the relationship after statistically adjusting for these powerful
and multidimensional selection processes. The analysis reveals a positive association between
incarceration and both current and lifetime psychiatric disorders, while helping to unpack its
underpinnings. Results indicate that (i) some of the most common disorders found among
former inmates emerge in childhood and adolescence; (ii) the effects of incarceration dissipate
somewhat over time, having a smaller impact on current disorders than lifetime disorders; and
(iii) substance disorders anticipate both other psychiatric disorders and incarceration. Yet the
results also reveal robust incarceration effects on certain disorders, which are no less
consequential for being specific. In particular, incarceration has a robust relationship with
subsequent mood disorders, related to feeling “down”, including major depressive disorder,
bipolar disorder, and dysthymia. These disorders, in turn, are strongly related to social and
economic disability. Indeed, mood disorders explain much of the additional social disability
former inmates experience following release. For those concerned with prisoner reintegration,
mood disorders may be an important—and generally neglected—consideration.

Out and Down:
The Effects of Incarceration on Psychiatric Disorders and Disability

Incarceration has risen dramatically over the last thirty years and sociologists are
beginning to understand its consequences for life chances and, more recently, health (Massoglia
and Schnittker 2009; Schnittker and John 2007; Wakefield and Uggen 2010). To date, social
scientists have documented many negative effects across multiple domains, but understanding
the effects on psychiatric disorders in particular presents a real opportunity for medical
sociologists to integrate seemingly disparate areas of research, including research on
stratification, criminalization, the life-course, and the sociology of mental health. Literatures on
the effects of institutionalization and stigma predicts negative effects of incarceration and, given
the effects of psychiatric disorders on disability, it is possible that psychiatric disorders explain at
least some fraction of the difficulties former inmates experience after their release. Certainly an
implicit theme of the reintegration literature is the difficulties former inmates experience in
trying to cope with their incarceration while simultaneously reassuming and renewing social
roles (Travis 2005), which aligns the literature with some research on psychiatric disability. Yet
the relationship between incarceration and psychiatric disorders also presents a particularly
complicated set of empirical concerns, especially related to the life-course dimensions of
incarceration. Both incarceration and psychiatric disorders are rooted in early childhood
experiences, potentially explaining their association in adulthood but undermining any effect of
incarceration. The criminalization of many common psychiatric disorders, especially related to
substance abuse, further underscores this possibility, suggesting many inmates end up in prison
1

at least partially as a result of their disorder. Beyond these considerations, it is not entirely clear
for which disorders incarceration might matter most, requiring a multidimensional approach to
outcomes, which is a hallmark of the sociological approach to mental health (Aneshensel, Rutter,
and Lachenbruch 1991).
Exploring the effects of incarceration on psychiatric disorders demands a great deal. In
this study we explore the relationship between lifetime incarceration and an array of common
psychiatric disorders, all while utilizing data sensitive to the life-course dimensions of psychiatric
disorders. In addition to exploring the effects of incarceration on psychiatric disorders, we also
examine whether such disorders help explain some fraction of incarceration’s other deleterious
social effects. We do so using the National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R), a
nationally representative survey of psychiatric disorders in the United States, which includes
elements necessary for a rigorous investigation, including a measure of disability spanning
cognitive, emotional, and social dimensions (Kessler et al. 2006; Kessler and Merikangas 2004;
Kessler and Ustun 2004). Understanding the effects of incarceration of mental health begins
with understanding the rise of incarceration.

The Rise of Incarceration and the Shadow of the Total Institution
The incarceration rate has increased precipitously in the United States of the last forty
years, leaving growing numbers of people with some lifetime contact with prisons or jails
(Wacquant 2009). In 1980, the incarceration rate was 149 per 100,000, whereas in 2009 the same
rate was five times as high at 749 (U.S. Department of Justice 2005; West 2010). The vast majority
2

of those in prison will be release, and all told, more than 700,000 people reenter their
communities from prison every year (West and Sabol 2009). Considering the stock and flow of
inmates, Uggen, Manza, and Thompson (2006) estimate that about 7.5% of the adult
population—approximately 16 million people—are felons or ex-felons, a figure that
approximates the number of unemployed persons during the economic recession of 2008 to 2009
(Wakefield and Uggen 2010). The rise of incarceration in the US may be unusual from many
standpoints, but incarceration is no longer uncommon and, if it is to be considered a type of
stress, medical sociologists should not consider it an especially rare one.
What, however, are the effects of incarceration on mental health? The potential relevance
of incarceration for mental health is large, but its effects are nevertheless uncertain and must be
appreciated in a broader epidemiological and institutional context. While there has been some
research on the effects of incarceration on mental health, much more research has been devoted
to the mental health of current inmates than the long-term consequences of prior incarceration,
which is potentially a more important topic given the churning of the prison system. Figure 1
provides a framework for understanding the effects of incarceration on the psychiatric disorders
and, in turn, the effects of psychiatric disorders on disability. The figure presents a number of
elements and pathways that will be considered shortly, but the immediate question is whether
there is a pathway from incarceration to psychiatric disorders, a pathway lying at the center of
the figure.
This pathway has received attention in the form of conceptual work on the effects of total
institutions more generally and from descriptive research on the prevalence of psychiatric
3

disorders in prisons. Goffman (1961) was perhaps the first to conceptually formalize the effects
of living in a total institution on mental health, but research on the social structure of prison life
predates his work (Weinberg 1942) and subsequent work has focused specifically on
prisonization as a particular form of coping (Sykes 1958; Wheeler 1961). More recent studies
document in a thorough way the many adjustments inmates make to life in prison and the
repercussions of those adjustments for well-being (Adams 1992; Bukstel and Kilmann 1980;
Haney 2003; Haney 2006). Whether reflected in Goffman work on total institutions or more
recent investigations on prisonization, the stress of life in prison remains clear: in being denied
their freedom, autonomy, features of their identity, and many goods and services, inmates often
suffer high levels of anxiety and distress. The prevalence of psychiatric disorders within prisons
is, by most accounts, relatively high (Fazel and Danesh 2002; Wilper et al. 2009). About one in
ten inmates experience major depression and, among male inmates, one in two experiences
antisocial personality disorder (Fazel and Danesh 2002). By some estimates, most returning
inmates exhibit some type of psychiatric disorder, even if a large fraction of those cases are
undiagnosed (Mallik-Kane and Visher 2008). Other studies estimate a diagnosed prevalence of
15 to 26 percent (Ditton 1999; Wilper et al. 2009), but characterize mental health problems as
“ubiquitous” relative to the general population (Wilper et al. 2009, p. 669), a conclusion echoed
in a report to Congress on the health of soon-to-be-released inmates, perhaps the most
comprehensive study of its kind (National Commission on Correctional Health Care 2002).
Although these studies focus on current inmates, the effects of incarceration on
psychiatric disorders are likely also related to experiences after release. A major theme of the
4

reintegration literature is the related to the difficulty of reintegration into traditional social roles,
rooted partly in discrimination (Pager 2008). If incarceration is causally related to these
difficulties and these difficulties are also related to psychiatric disorders, then the total effects of
incarceration will reflect both experiences occurring within prison and experiences resulting
from a prior prison sentence. Consistent with this intuition, research on the health effects of
incarceration finds the length of incarceration is only weakly related to health after adjusting for
whether someone spent any time in prison (Massoglia 2008; Schnittker and John 2007).
From a descriptive standpoint the high prevalence of psychiatric disorders among current
and former inmates seems clear, but it is unclear this elevated prevalence reflects. The problem
stems in large part from the focus of many studies: most of the conceptual research focuses on
life in the total institution and much of the descriptive work focuses on the mental health of
current inmates, but research on the social origins of crime and psychiatric disorders focuses
increasingly on the early developmental antecedents of each. Figure 1 presents these influences
to the left of incarceration, exerting an influence on incarceration, but also outcomes further
down the pathway. One important feature of the figure is the centrality of childhood
disadvantage to many of the outcomes appearing on the right. To the degree that childhood
disadvantage is associated with both incarceration and adult psychiatric disorders, the apparent
relationship between the two may be confounded and there is, in fact, considerable evidence
linking childhood disadvantage to a host of behavioral problems. Childhood adversities have
been linked to many types of psychiatric disorders throughout adulthood (Green et al. 2010) and,

5

likewise, have been linked to the early onset of delinquency and the stability of criminal behavior
over the life course (Sampson and Laub 1992).
Moving to the right of childhood disadvantage, it is also the case that many psychiatric
disorders emerge early in life and will predate adult incarceration (Kessler et al. 2005; Kessler and
Wang 2008; Paus, Keshavan, and Giedd 2008). Given the early average onset of most psychiatric
disorders, many disorders found among adults actually reflect recurrent or chronic disorders
and, therefore, reflect much earlier epidemiological processes. Indeed, this is perhaps especially
true among former inmates, as some of the most common psychiatric disorders found among
inmates have unusually early onsets. For example, most impulse control disorders, characterized
by a predisposition toward swift action in pursuit of gratification with little regard for long-term
negative consequences (Moeller et al. 2001), begin in childhood (Kessler and Wang 2008).
Findings of this sort are consistent with Gottfredson and Hirschi’s (1990) general theory of
crime, which locates the root cause of criminality in the failure to develop self-control early in
life, leading to numerous subsequent problems, including criminal behavior and psychiatric
disorders. Of course, not all criminal behavior reflects a psychiatric disorder and not all inmates
are mentally ill. Nevertheless, these epidemiological patterns suggest that those with a history of
incarceration may have distinct psychiatric patterns regarding age of onset and chronicity and,
furthermore, these patterns suggest that incarceration itself exerts little causal influence.
A related complication stems from patterns of comorbidity between substance-specific
disorders and other psychiatric disorders. An important finding of contemporary descriptive
epidemiology is that many commonly occurring disorders are comorbid with others (Kessler and
6

Wang 2008) and an especially common pairing is between substance disorders and mood/anxiety
disorders (Kessler, Chiu, Demler, and Walters 2005). This pattern has wide-ranging implications
for understanding the epidemiology of psychiatric conditions, but it has some specific
implications for those interested in the effects of incarceration. Because many crimes either
reflect the behavioral disinhibition associated with drug/alcohol use or are a direct reflection of
possessing controlled substances (Felson 1994), the prevalence of some disorders among
current/former inmates might simply reflect the co-occurrence of these disorders with directly
criminalized conditions and behaviors (Abram and Teplin 1991). Along these lines, Swartz and
Lurigio (2007) find that the relationship between arrest and serious mental illness can be
explained either entirely or substantially by substance use, depending on the offense. Early-onset
substance abuse is related to the subsequent onset of a variety of other disorders, as well as
delinquency and criminal behavior (Ellickson, Tucker, and Klein 2003; McCarty et al. 2004). For
this reason, it is important to consider early-onset substance use disorders even when
considering the effects of incarceration on other disorders, as substance-use disorders could play
a direct role in both incarceration and those disorders.
Although none of the influences depicted on the left-side of Figure 1 suggests that the
relationship between incarceration and psychiatric disorders is entirely spurious, they do suggest
that those interested in identifying the influence of incarceration must control for a variety of
risk factors anterior to incarceration and adult psychiatric conditions. It is also clear that
researchers must consider a variety of outcomes. A distinct feature of the sociological approach
to mental health is to consider effects across a range of outcomes and, thus, seriously consider
7

whether the effects of stress are disorder specific or general, as is usually anticipated in stress
research (Aneshensel, Rutter, and Lachenbruch 1991). This feature is especially important in the
case of incarceration as the outcomes potentially affected by incarceration vary greatly in terms
of age of onset, with many disorders having an average onset well prior to adulthood, and in
terms of their relationships with crime and criminality, with some disorders criminalized directly
(e.g., drug abuse) or related to criminal behavior (e.g., alcohol abuse). Beyond these issues of the
antecedents of psychiatric disorders, the rightmost side of Figure 1 suggests that researchers
should also consider the consequences of psychiatric disorders for other outcomes, including
reintegration.
Psychiatric Disorders and the Difficulties of Reintegration
Current research on incarceration tends to focus on its social and economic
consequences, rather than its consequences for health (Pager 2008; Wakefield and Uggen 2010;
Western 2007). However the two areas are not unrelated and, indeed, it is possible that
incarceration’s psychological effects shape incarceration’s social consequences, even if most
research fails to make the connection explicit. A good deal of evidence points to the difficulties of
reintegration, whether in terms of employment, family, or community. Former inmates are, for
example, less likely to be employed and earn less money when they are working (Pager 2003;
Western 2002). They are also less likely to be married and have a difficult time sustaining
relationships through a period of incarceration and after (Western and Wildeman 2009). These
difficulties are usually interpreted in terms of human capital or stigma, as when former inmates
are not hired because of their interrupted work histories, when state laws regulate their eligibility
8

for certain occupations, or when employers engage in outright discrimination. These influences
are demonstrably important, but a number of scholars argue that mental health problems pose
another important and neglected barrier to reintegration (Mallik-Kane and Visher 2008;
Pogorzelski, Wolff, Pan, and Blitz 2005), citing the strong relationship between psychiatric
disorders and disability in social, economic, and cognitive domains (Merikangas et al. 2007;
Ormel et al. 1994). Psychiatric disorders affect disability by impairing higher-order capacities
involved in virtually all daily activities (Ormel et al. 1994) and, for this reason, the effects of
psychiatric disorders on disability may exceed those of physical disorders (Merikangas et al.
2007). Here, too, it is important to consider the specificity of incarceration’s effects. Of the
different disorders potentially experienced by former inmates, substance abuse may play a
particularly strong role, but other conditions could contribute as well, including mood and
anxiety disorders, given their unusually strong effects on disability (Merikangas et al. 2007). As
before, it is important to consider what specific disorders incarceration may be related to when
considering whether psychiatric disorders explain some part of the relationship between
incarceration and disability.
Summary and Data Requirements
This study has three interrelated goals: (i) to understand the effects of incarceration in a
life-course framework sensitive to the many forces that affect incarceration and psychiatric
disorders simultaneously; (ii) to consider the role of psychiatric disorders in inhibiting
reintegration by exploring the effects of incarceration and disorders on disability; and, in the
context of both of these goals, (iii) to consider multiple and varied psychiatric outcomes
9

simultaneously, thereby delimiting in a precise fashion the range of incarceration’s potential
psychological effects.

The National Comorbidity Survey Replication
The National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R) is a nationally-representative
survey and the benchmark source for current information on psychiatric disorders in the United
States. It was carried out between 2001 and 2003 within the coterminous states among
respondents age 18 and older (Alegria, Jackson, Kessler, and Takeuchi 2008; Kessler et al. 2006;
Kessler and Merikangas 2004). The NCS-R was administered face-to-face using computerassisted personal interview methods, which mitigates some of the difficulties of administering an
unusually long and complex instrument containing numerous questions about potentially
sensitive topics. The overall sample size was 9,282 and the response rate was over 70 percent. The
primary purpose of the NCS-R was to assess change in the prevalence and correlates of
psychiatric disorders. To this end, it followed the original NCS, which was administered in the
early 1990s, and repeated many of the original questions, updated to reflect changes in
psychiatric nomenclature (Kessler and Merikangas 2004).
The instrument used in the NCS-R was divided into two parts, each reflecting a different
goal. Part I was administered to all respondents (N=9,282) and contained questions about the
core disorders included in the World Mental Health Initiative Version of the Composite
International Diagnostic Instrument (WMH-CIDI), discussed in more detail below. Part II
contained information on risk factors and other correlates of psychiatric disorders, as well as
10

questions about disorders that were not of primary interest or were difficult to assess and,
therefore, not included in Part I. Given the number of questions required to assess even the
common disorders evaluated in Part I, Part II was not administered to all respondents. It was,
instead, administered to respondents revealed in Part I to have psychiatric disorders or
significant symptoms (i.e., those who met the criteria for a lifetime disorder; who met
subthreshold criteria and received some kind of treatment; or who had made a plan to commit
suicide) and a probability subsample of other respondents (N=5,692). Because our study is
concerned with some key variables contained only in Part II, we limit the analysis to Part II
respondents, adjusting for survey design, non-response, and sample selection using Part IIspecific sampling weights.
Part II contains the core demographic control variables used in our study, as well as the
disability assessment. Our primary variable of interest is, of course, incarceration. Respondents
were asked whether they ever spent time in prison, jail, or a correctional facility since the age of
18. Our models include a dichotomous indicator: whether or not the respondent was ever
incarcerated. Approximately 14 percent of respondents reported having been in prison/jail in
their lifetime, but because of an unusual skip pattern, not all eligible respondents were asked this
question. Respondents who reported that their religious beliefs were “not at all important” in
their daily life were not asked additional about questions about their religiosity, but, more
important for our purposes, they were also not asked questions about incarceration (among
other background questions not used here). This skip pattern is unusual and unfortunate, but it
has little apparent consequence for our specific research questions and the few missing cases it
11

introduces can be recaptured using an increasingly popular and well-established modeling
procedure. First, very few respondents reported that religion was not at all important in their
lives (less than 8 percent), meaning few cases are actually missing because of the skip pattern.
Second, the response category adjacent to “not at all important” is quite similar in denotation
(“not very important”), allowing us to test whether the effects of incarceration on psychiatric
disorders varied between the meaningfully different levels of religiosity that were observed
among those reporting any incarceration (in this case spanning “not very important” and “very
important”). Interactions of this sort could reveal that the average effects estimated in our sample
diverge from those we might find in a more demonstrably representative sample, but these
interactions were almost entirely insignificant with no more significant interactions than
expected by chance (8 percent) and, even among these few significant interactions, only half
revealed linear patterns vis-à-vis adjacent categories, suggesting random variation.
Although there were few missing cases in general, all the results presented in the paper
were derived from multiply-imputed data. Multiple imputation is a multiple-step process
wherein missing values are predicted using a statistical model based on all the observed
covariates, after which data sets are generated using expected values generated from the model
while accounting for sampling variability by imputing over multiple data sets (Allison 2001; Little
and Rubin 2002). From this it is possible to estimate models using each of the data sets and then
use combination rules to merge these separate models into a single set of results (Rubin 1987).
Imputing in this fashion over twenty data sets, we recapture a complete set of 5,692 cases rather
than 5,204. To assure the robustness of our results, we also, produced results using listwise
12

deletion (tables available on request). There were no significant differences between the imputed
data and listwise deletion data either in terms of summary statistics or regression results—
indeed, in many cases, the prevalence was identical to the digit—with one notable exception.
Using multiply imputed data increased the lifetime prevalence of alcohol abuse among those with
a history of incarceration to 47.1% from 16%. Despite this increase, the multiply imputed
regression models using alcohol abuse remained very similar to the listwise deletion regression
models.
World Mental Health Version of the Composite International Diagnostic Interview
The primary diagnostic interview schedule used in the NCS-R was the World Mental
Health Version of the Composite International Diagnostic Interview (WMH-CIDI) (Kessler and
Ustun 2004). The WMH-CIDI is a fully-structured diagnostic interview that generates diagnoses
consistent with the criteria contained in, for our study, the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of
American Psychiatric Association version 4 (DSM-IV). The WMH-CIDI generates both lifetime
and 12-month diagnoses, the former indicating those who experienced a given disorder at any
period in the their lifetime and the latter indicating those who experienced a lifetime disorder
and had significant symptoms consistent with the disorder in the preceding 12 months. The
WMH-CIDI is intended for lay administration, meaning that those who meet the criteria for a
disorder need not have been diagnosed by a clinician. Despite this, clinical reappraisal studies
reveal that the WMH-CIDI shows reasonably good concordance with structured clinical
interviews (Kessler, Chiu, Demler, and Walters 2005). Moreover, at least some of the discordance
between the two likely reflects the unreliability of clinical interviews, rather than the unreliability
13

of the WMH-CIDI. Lay interviews are perhaps even more essential for our study than for many
other applications. First, by using a fully-structured interview format, the WMH-CIDI avoids
potential contamination between diagnostic decisions and a clinician’s knowledge of a person’s
imprisonment, which can lead to a significant clinical bias (Rhodes 2000; Rhodes 2002). Second,
the fully-structured interview format avoids contamination between diagnoses and self-reported
treatments. Many former inmates may be reluctant to seek services for fear of appearing “weak,”
leading to an especially strong disjuncture between diagnoses based on self-reports or treatment
seeking and disorders derived only from clinical criteria (Mallik-Kane and Visher 2008).
There are other particular benefits to the NCS-R, notably an expanded set of diagnoses.
Unlike the earlier NCS, the NCS-R assesses child and adolescent disorders, as well as assorted
impulse-control disorders that are closely correlated with criminal behavior (Kessler and
Merikangas 2004). In addition, compared with earlier diagnostic criteria, the DSM-IV is
diagnostically conservative: it places greater emphasis on clinically significant distress and
impairment, meaning that studies employing its criteria rather than those of earlier editions
generally find lower prevalences. A final feature of the WMH-CIDI deserves comment. Below we
use information on the reported age of onset, which is essential to modeling social selection.
Given the uncertainty of most psychiatric symptoms, it is difficult to recall precisely when a
disorder began, but the NCS-R made a special effort to increase the accuracy of respondent recall
and, indeed, found improvements over previous instruments (Knäuper et al. 1999).
Childhood Adversities

14

The NCS-R assessed a variety of childhood adversities occurring before age 18, divided
into four types based on prior analyses (Green et al. 2010). Most of these items are premised on
an instrument created for the original NCS (Kessler, Davis, and Kendler 1997). Parental
maladjustment is the sum of four indicators: mental illness, substance abuse, criminality
(whether either parent engaged in criminal behavior or was arrested), or violence. Interpersonal
loss is the sum of three indicators: parental death, divorce, or other separation from parents or
caregivers. Abuse or neglect is the sum of three indicators: physical abuse, sexual abuse, and
neglect. And economic adversity is whether the respondent’s family ever received welfare. These
items were culled from a variety of sources, including the first wave of the NCS (Kessler, Davis,
and Kendler 1997) and related surveys (Courtney, Piliavin, Grogan-Kaylor, and Nesmith 1998),
the Family History Research Diagnostic Criteria Interview (Endicott, Andreasen, and Spitzer
1978; Kendler et al. 1991), and the Conflict Tactics Scales (Straus 1979).
World Health Organization Disability Assessment Schedule
Part II respondents were also administered the World Health Organization Disability
Assessment Schedule (WHO-DAS) (Rehm et al. 1999). The WHO-DAS was designed to
measure functional impairments within the last 30 days across six dimensions derived from the
WHO’s International Classification of Impairment, Disabilities, and Handicaps. The domains
and their questions are: (i) role loss, defined as the number of days in which the respondent was
unable to complete normal activities; (ii) self-care limitations, defined as the number of days in
which they had difficulty with washing, getting dressed, and staying alone; (iii) mobility
limitations, defined as the number of days they had difficulty standing for 30 minutes, moving
15

inside the house, and walking a long distance; (iv) cognition, defined as the number of days they
had difficulty concentrating for 10 minutes, understanding what was going on, remembering to
do important things, and learning a new task; (v) social functioning, defined as the number of
days they had difficulty getting alone with others, maintaining a conversation, dealing with
people they did not know, maintaining friendships, making new friends, and controlling
emotions around other people; and, (vi) social participation, defined as the amount of
embarrassment and discrimination due to health problems. Per convention, each of the domains
was scaled to a theoretical range of zero to 100, with zero indicating no disability and 100
indicating complete disability within the domain.
The WHO-DAS is concerned with health-related disability as a matter of focus, but it
uses an expansive approach. For example, “health” includes mental and physical disorders. In
addition, the WHO-DAS is a “global” or “external” measure of disability, rather than a “specific”
or “internal” measure, in that it asks about general health-related disability regardless of the
specific source. Although internal evaluations are useful and can speak readily to the relative
contributions of different disorders—a topic we are interested in for theoretical reasons—
internal evaluations require the respondent to report the amount of disability directly
attributable to a single disorder. This is difficult for a number of reasons, not least of which is the
high degree of comorbidity among different disorders and the difficulty of determining cause
and effect amidst overlapping biological processes. Given this, we will identify the relative
contributions of different disorders inferentially, estimating regression models that attempt to
distill the specific sources of disability while controlling for all others. Making accurate inferences
16

about the relative contributions of psychiatric disorders necessitates measuring other health
conditions, including chronic physical conditions that are comorbid with psychiatric disorders
and entail significant disability. The chronic conditions checklist included in the NCS-R was
adapted from the list used in the National Health Interview Survey. Most of the items in this list
reference whether a physician diagnosed the condition (e.g., has a doctor ever diagnosed you
with hypertension) and, for this reason, the checklist will underestimate conditions that are not
brought to the attention of a professional. But the NCS-R also contains questions about
symptoms that are impairing but not well-matched with a disorder, such as unexplained chronic
pain. In the disability models, we include the sum of eight chronic conditions shown to be related
to disability: arthritis, back pain, head ache, chronic pain, stroke, asthma, chronic obstructed
pulmonary disease, and epilepsy.
Analytic Map and Empirical Considerations
In what follows, we explore the relationship between incarceration and psychiatric
disorders over six tables, each of which builds on those preceding it. Table 1 begins with simple
prevalence estimates, presenting the prevalence of lifetime and 12-month disorders for those
with and without a history of incarceration, allowing us to assess whether there is a positive
association between incarceration and psychiatric disorders. We use both lifetime and 12-month
disorders, although 12-month disorders provide a better test of the long-term effects of lifetime
incarceration than lifetime disorders and, thus, are the focus of some of our later analyses. Table
2 considers the characteristics of psychiatric disorders in greater detail, assessing whether those
with a history of incarceration have earlier onset disorders, implying longer standing disorders,
17

and whether those with a history of incarceration have more chronic disorders, implying more
severe cases. We present the average age of onset for each disorder, as well as the fraction of 12month cases to lifetime cases, an indicator of the persistence of each disorder (a perfectly
persistent disorder will yield a ratio of one, whereas a perfectly remitting disorder will, over a
sufficient long period of time, yield a ratio of zero). Again we present these figures for those with
and without a history of incarceration and present statistical tests of the difference.
The remaining tables attempt to discern the effects of incarceration and consequences of
psychiatric disorders for disability. Table 3 presents basic descriptive statistics of the elements
depicted on the left-side of Figure 1, childhood adversity and early onset substance abuse. Tables
4 and 5 combine the preceding tables in a multivariate regression context, exploring the effects of
incarceration on lifetime (Table 4) and 12-month (Table 5) psychiatric disorders. Each table
presents four models. In Model 1 we estimate the relationship between incarceration and
psychiatric disorders including only demographic control variables either plausibly anterior to
the process (i.e., education) or additively unrelated to causal considerations (i.e., race/ethnicity,
age, and sex) (although we do test for interactions and discuss these briefly). In Model 2, we add
childhood background and in Model 3 we add early-onset substance disorders. In the final
model, we adjust for the early onset of the disorder under consideration, either by dropping cases
whose onset was prior to age 18 (in the case of lifetime disorders) or simply controlling for under
18 onset (in the case of 12-month disorders). Table 6 explores whether psychiatric disorders

18

mediate the association between incarceration and disability.1 We do so in three models, the
second adjusting for education and the third including counts of assorted disorder types, along
with a count of other chronic physical conditions. The second model allows us to compare the
role of psychiatric disorders with that of education, whose relationship with disability and
incarceration is more established.
Heterogeneity in the Effects of Incarceration
The tables focus on average effects for the entire sample, but a number of additional
specifications were estimated. As noted, incarceration is more common among men than
women and among African Americans than whites. In supplementary analyses, we estimated
interactions between incarceration and race/ethnicity and incarceration and sex. Of the 90
possible interactions with race (three interactions, exhausting comparisons between four
racial/ethnic groups), only 1 was significant, less than expected by chance (incarceration has
more of a relationship with social phobia among “other” racial/ethnic groups). Similarly, of the
1

Modeling disability presents particular challenges. To this point, the results will be presented in
terms of specific disorders, but we move to a summary count measure when estimating the
effects of disorders on disability. Specifically we model psychiatric disorders as counts within
four specific DSM categories: anxiety disorders, mood disorders, impulse control disorders, and
substance disorders. We do so for several reasons. First, modeling the influence of disorders
using counts constrains the influence of each disorder to be equal, but it circumvents the
contaminating influence of multicollinearity, which can be severe especially among disorders of
the same type. Second, other studies exploring specific permutations of comorbidity reveal that
comorbidity between disorders generally produces no greater disability than expected from a
simple additive model of those disorders, meaning that a simple count is an empirically accurate
way to account for the complexity of multiple disorders (Merikangas et al. 2007). To account for
comorbidity with physical disorders, we also control for the number of chronic physical health
problems, as described above. Most research suggests that models that fail to account for
comorbidity among physical and psychological disorders risk overstating the effects of one or the
other, even if the effects of psychiatric disorders are generally stronger than those of physical
disorders.
19

30 possible interactions with sex, only 2 were significant, both suggesting a stronger negative
effect for women than men (for lifetime intermittent explosive disorder and 12-month adult
separation anxiety disorder). Although these insignificant interactions are inconsistent with the
intuition of group differences in the stress of incarceration, they are not inconsistent with other
health research, which also finds insignificant or inconsistently significant interactions
(Schnittker and John 2007).
By the same token we focus on the effects being ever incarcerated, but incarceration
varies greatly in its length. The mean for the NCS-R is 162 days, but the median is just under a
week. In supplementary models we explored the relationship between length of incarceration
and psychiatric disorders using semi-parametric methods, ultimately focusing on those disorders
for which we found the strongest effects (discussed shortly). Like other studies, these models
revealed that the length of a sentence was largely unrelated to psychiatric outcomes beyond the
difference between those with and without a history of incarceration (Massoglia 2008; Schnittker
and John 2007). If anything, the effect of sentence length was non-linear, such that it increased
slowly up to 2 to 3 years, but declined thereafter. In this light, modeling the effects of
incarceration as a dummy-variable is appropriate, but it is important to remember the relatively
small sample of those with unusually long sentences and, further, the great heterogeneity among
those with sentences of less than a year. Some respondents with less than a year of a total time
reflect those doing multiple short sentences, some one long sentence, some in local facilities, and
some in state facilities. The conditions of incarceration are almost certainly related to

20

incarceration’s effects, but these conditions are only imperfectly correlated with the duration of a
sentence.

Results
Table 1 reveals pervasive differences in the prevalence of psychiatric disorders between
those with and without a history of incarceration, both in terms of lifetime and 12-month
disorders. The largest differentials are found with respect to substance use disorders. Not
surprisingly, as many as 29 percent of former inmates have met the criteria for drug abuse during
their lifetime and nearly half of those with a history of incarceration abused alcohol. Yet the
prevalence of psychiatric disorders is elevated across the full spectrum of disorders: former
inmates are more likely to experience anxiety disorders, mood disorders, and impulse control
disorders. Former inmates also have a higher prevalence of current disorders. In the case of 12month disorders, the most common disorders are phobias. In addition, major depressive
disorder is more common than either alcohol dependence, drug abuse, or drug dependence,
despite the link between the latter and crime. All three mood disorders are more common than
either oppositional defiant disorder or conduct disorder. Table 1 provides preliminary evidence
that directly criminalized conditions are not the only relevant disorders when considering former
inmates.
—Insert Table 1 About Here—
Table 2 provides the first bridge to a more rigorous analysis. Although the prevalence of
psychiatric disorders may be higher among former inmates, other characteristics of psychiatric
21

disorders are not consistently different. Table 2 explores the average age of onset for each of the
disorders, along with persistence. For our purposes, this table is relevant for at least three
reasons: it explores whether the disorders described in Table 1 emerge before adult
incarceration, whether the age of onset is earlier for those with a history of incarceration, and
whether psychiatric disorders are more persistent among former inmates, which we might expect
if the disorders found among former inmates are qualitatively different from those found among
others. For five of the eighteen disorders, former inmates have first-onsets at significantly
younger ages, as expected by those who emphasize the relationship between childhood
disadvantage and early onset. Yet for most disorders there is no difference between formers
inmates and others, and the few significant differences occasionally are in the opposite direction.
For drug dependence, for example, former inmates have a significantly later onset. As has been
established elsewhere, mood disorders generally have the latest onset, for former inmates and
others alike. The persistence of psychiatric disorders is also, in general, not different between
former inmates and others. Former inmates have significantly more persistent cases of
agoraphobia and intermittent explosive disorder, as indicated by a higher percent of 12-month
cases to lifetime cases. But none of the remaining differences are significant and, in the case of all
four substance disorders, former inmates actually have slightly less persistent cases, as might be
the case if former inmates are required, for example, to undergo drug treatment. In general,
Table 2 reveals only a few differences that are, in themselves, insufficient to suggest that the
psychiatric disorders experienced by former inmates are of a qualitatively different sort from
those experienced by those without a history of incarceration.
22

—Insert Table 2 About Here—
The origins of their disorders may, however, be distinct. Table 3 presents summary
statistics for the childhood background control variables and reveals pervasive differences.
Former inmates are more likely to experience interpersonal loss, family maladaption, economic
adversity, and abuse or neglect. They are also much more likely to experience early onset drug or
alcohol abuse, with 15% of former inmates abusing alcohol before the age of 18 and 11% abusing
drugs. These risk factors may explain some and perhaps all of the association between adult
incarceration and adult psychiatric disorders, which we explore in Tables 4 and 5.
—Insert Table 3 About Here—
In both Tables 4 and 5, we consider only those conditions for which adult onset is
diagnostically possible, which eliminates three of the four impulse control disorders discussed
earlier, but we reconsider these disorders when discussing disability, as they could explain some
of the disability former inmates experience even if they are not a consequence of incarceration.
We also do not consider substance disorders to a great extent, given their status in most of our
models as control variables. Table 4 begins with lifetime disorders and Table 5 continues with
12-month disorders. The models are presented in rows, rather than the conventional columns,
but proceed left to right through progressively more stringent specifications, as discussed above.
Within the rows the coefficients correspond to the incarceration coefficient from each logit
model.
—Insert Tables 4 and 5 About Here—

23

Although they use different outcomes, tables 4 and 5 yield similar conclusions. First, the
relationship between incarceration and psychiatric disorders is highly sensitive to control
variables. Not all the control variables are equal in their relevance. For all the psychiatric
disorders and for both lifetime and 12-month disorders the percentage reduction in the
coefficients is greater between models 1 and 2 than between 2 and 3, meaning childhood
background plays a more important role than early onset substance abuse in shaping the
relationship between incarceration and psychiatric disorders. The influence of childhood
background is remarkably consistent across disorders, accounting for 26% of the coefficient in
Model 1 for lifetime disorders and 28% of the coefficient for 12-month disorders. Proceeding to
the final model, which includes all the control variables, most of the association between
incarceration and psychiatric disorders remains, but considering consistency between both tables
4 and 5, the relationship between incarceration and mood disorders remains surprisingly robust.
Incarceration more than doubles the odds of current dysthymia and increases the odds of major
depression by nearly 50%. There are also assorted relationships between incarceration and
certain anxiety disorders—notably, for example, post-traumatic stress disorder—but none of the
relationships that is significant for lifetime disorders is also significant for 12-month disorders
and vice versa. In sum, the evidence for an effect of incarceration on most psychiatric disorders
is weak, but we cannot rule out effects on mood disorders and these effects, although specific, are
generally strong.2

The models present average effects for all former inmates implicitly weighted by the prevalence
of incarceration, but the demographic composition of prisons is unusual and the average effect
may mask meaningful heterogeneity. In supplementary models we tested interactions between

2

24

Table 6 reveals these effects are also consequential. The table presents three models for
each of the six forms of disability evaluated in the WHO-DAS. The first model presents the
relationship between incarceration and disability with basic controls; the second model adds
education in order to evaluate the explanatory power of a popular human capital indicator
strongly related to incarceration, as discussed above; and the third model adds psychiatric
disorders and chronic physical conditions. The results reveal, first, that former inmates suffer
from a great deal of disability, manifest across multiple dimensions. The relationship between
incarceration and disability is significant in all six cases, and in four of the cases the difference
between those with and without a history of incarceration exceeds the difference between those
with 16 or more years of education and those without a high school diploma. The table also
reveals, however, that the problems inmates experience after release do not stem from human
capital deficits alone (or even primarily). The difference between models 2 and 3 reveals that
mental and physical health problems explain anywhere from 32 percent (mobility disability) to
88 percent (cognitive disability) of the relationship between incarceration and disability. Notably
in the case of self-care and cognitive disability, the association between incarceration and
disability is explained entirely in Model 3. Perhaps even more important, a large fraction of this
mediation stems from mood disorders specifically. Across the six different types of disability, the

race and incarceration and sex and incarceration, seeing in particular if over-represented groups
(e.g., African Americans, men) differed significantly from under-represented ones (e.g., whites,
women) (interactions were added to the model with age-of-onset controls). The vast majority of
these coefficients were insignificant, including all the race interactions. In the case of sex, seven
interactions were significant and positive, but most of the interactions were for lifetime disorders
(i.e., agoraphobia, post-traumatic stress disorder, adult separation anxiety, dysthymia, and
bipolar disorder). None of the 12-month mood disorder interactions was significant.
25

consequences of mood disorders exceed those of conditions tightly associated with incarceration
by virtue of criminalization, including impulse control disorders and substance disorders, which
matter very little. Of the twelve coefficients associated with the latter two types of disorder, only
one is significant and, furthermore, with the exception of mobility disability, mood disorders
play a more important role than chronic physical health conditions. Mood disorders are
particularly important in explaining social interaction and social participation disabilities, which
are important in their own right be especially because of their relationship with the success of
prisoner reintegration.
—Insert Table 6 About Here—

Discussion
A long-standing line of sociological research is concerned with the enduring effects of
total institutions on mental health and life chances, but we know surprisingly little about the
mental health of former inmates (although we know a little about the mental health of current
inmates) and very little research has attempted to deal with the many potential threats to a causal
claim, including the criminalization of many psychiatric disorders and the origins of both
criminal behavior and psychiatric disorders in childhood adversities. The literatures on total
institutions, prisoner reintegration, the life-course dimensions of offending, and the sociology of
mental health remain largely segregated, even though there is considerable overlap in their
themes.

26

The results of this study suggest that effects of incarceration are both more and less than
anticipated. Although former inmates have higher rates of psychiatric disorder for virtually all
common disorders, in most instances the association does not reflect an effect of incarceration.
Precisely because of the overlap between childhood conditions and both offending and
psychiatric disorders, the relationship between incarceration and many psychiatric disorders is
highly sensitive to childhood background factors and, to a lesser degree, early-onset substance
abuse. In this way, our results highlight the considerable overlap between the life-course
determinants of crime and the life-course determinants of psychiatric outcomes: both are a
reflection of childhood adversities. Incarceration does, however, have robust effects on mood
disorders and, for this class of disorders, the effects are quite strong. Incarceration increases the
odds of lifetime major depression—the most common psychiatric disorder in the general
population (Kessler et al. 2005)—by 33 percent. The effects on 12-month dysthymia are even
stronger, where incarceration more than doubles the odds. Although the relationship between
incarceration and 12-month bipolar disorder is not significant at conventional levels, the
coefficient from the most rigorous specification (Model 4) is in fact larger than the significant
coefficient found in a less rigorous specification (Model 3). It is possible that a larger sample
would produce more consistent findings, and in this vein it is notable that many of the
incarceration coefficients for anxiety disorders are quite large, even if statistically insignificant.
Nevertheless, our empirical confidence is limited to mood disorders, and even if the effects of
incarceration are limited in this fashion, they are no less important for being particular.

27

Mood disorders are strongly related to disability and play an important role in explaining
some the difficulties former inmates experience after release relative to those without a history of
incarceration. Indeed, our results suggest that different between former inmates and others in
disability could be reduced greatly or, in two cases, eliminated entirely by addressing psychiatric
disorders. The mediation of the incarceration coefficient is driven primarily by mood disorders,
whose relationship with disability exceeds that of almost all the other condition categories,
whether mental or physical. This set of findings is important for several reasons. For one, it
suggests that our intuitions regarding what matters for selection into prison are not a particularly
good guide for our predictions regarding what is most consequential following release (see also
Uggen and Piliavin 1998 on asymmetric causation). Although substance disorders and impulsecontrol disorders are among the most common disorders found among former inmates and have
direct relevance to criminal behavior, they are not the most relevant for understanding disability
and, by extension, reintegration. This asymmetric causation illustrates one specific way in which
the segregation of the literatures related to the topic of incarceration and mental health may
come at a cost: focusing only on childhood disadvantage or social selection or reintegration may
miss how processes are related. By the same token, only by considering multiple psychiatric
outcomes is it possible to discern what might be a consequence of incarceration and what might
be a determinant, a point that may be missed by those focusing on “mental health” in general.
This finding is also relevant to social policy, especially to those responsible for providing
services to returning inmates. But here, too, the findings introduce questions regarding whether
existing theoretical frameworks are appropriate for formulating programs. Along these lines,
28

there have been some efforts among service providers to more closely align the criminal justice
system with the public health system, with some arguing that by protecting the health and wellbeing of former inmates we can also protect the health and safety of the community at large
(Freudenberg et al. 2008; Golembeski and Fullilove 2005). One implication of our study is that
such programs may also serve to promote reintegration, but mood disorders—the focus of our
claims—do not fit comfortably into existing criminal-justice-as-public-health frameworks. Mood
disorders are not directly criminalized, like substance abuse disorders, nor are they infectious,
like HIV/AIDS or tuberculosis. For these reasons, mood disorders may fall through the cracks of
even the most broad and progressive frameworks: they may be seen as less relevant to the
experience of former inmates or less amenable to treatment and, thus, not be a typical part of
needs assessment. Our results suggest, to the contrary, that they should be an important element
of service delivery because they have direct implications for reintegration and, thus, might even
be considered an important element in parole.
The results also point to areas of mutual interest between those concerned with the health
consequences of incarceration and those interested in finding mechanisms to ameliorate
incarceration’s pervasive negative effects. Although stigma and discrimination play a
demonstrable role in employment and marriage among former inmates, psychiatric disorders are
also relevant to these outcomes but they are generally not part of the debate. They are important
in a number of respects. For one, it is possible that stigma and mental illness work hand-in-hand
in the sense that the stigma of incarceration overlaps with the stigma of mental illness. For
example, employers may be reluctant to hire former inmates because they perceive them to be
29

mentally ill, much as employers are reluctant to hire former mental patients because they are
perceived to be dangerous (Link and Phelan 2001). Mood disorders may also be significant in
their own right, regardless of stigma, meaning that even if discrimination against former inmates
were reduced or eliminated, former inmates would continue to suffer because of their mood
disorders. Indeed, because of their strong effects on disability related to functioning and
cognition, mood disorders provide scholars with a mechanism for explaining the self-defeating
behavior of former inmates, a task for which discrimination alone is likely ill-suited. For
example, there may be something to the claim that former inmates are discriminated against in
part because they are believed to be poor employees (Pager, Western, and Bonikowski 2009), but
insofar as this is true at all, the diminished skills of former inmates may have more to do with
their poor mental health than with diminished motivation, intelligence, or organization. By
recognizing the consequences of mood disorders, it is possible to develop a more robust
appreciation of former inmates’ behavior and recognize some of its structural determinants.
Although our analysis focuses on the unique effects of incarceration, it is important to
note that the disorders experienced by former inmates do not appear to be much different from
those experienced by others. It is true that psychiatric disorders among former inmates often
begin in childhood and adolescence, but this is true for those without a history of incarceration as
well (Paus, Keshavan, and Giedd 2008). Similarly, the effects of incarceration may be enduring
in the sense that they are found for both current and lifetime disorders, but it is not the case that
former inmates suffer from more persistent disorders. The ratio differences presented in Table 2
are significant in only 2 of 18 cases and, for those disorders more consistently linked to
30

incarceration across all the model specifications, none of the ratios is significant. Furthermore,
the effects of incarceration on lifetime disorders are, in most cases, more powerful than those on
12-month disorders, implying that the negative effects of incarceration, such as they are, are not
necessarily enduring and that many disorders among former inmates are self-limiting, much as
they are in the population in general (Kessler and Wang 2008). In short, while psychiatric
disorders are more prevalent among inmates than others, they may be just as amenable to
treatment as they are among those without a history of incarceration.
Limitations
The strengths of this study stem from the NCS-R and how the survey allows the analyst to
explore psychiatric disorders with breadth and precision, a benefit found in few nationally
representative surveys, but the study’s weaknesses reflect the flip side of the same coin. Although
we observe many influences relevant to understanding the effects of incarceration, these
influences are often measured with error. The age-of-onset controls, for example, involve
respondent retrospection, which is of course imperfect, especially for disorders not often
associated with a discernable event (e.g., a heart attack). Similarly, childhood adversities are
based on retrospective reports and may be underestimated or correlated with current mental
health (Kendler et al. 1991). Nonetheless, there are signs that our control variables do, indeed,
capture the many of the influences we are concerned with. If selection were the preeminent force
behind the observed relationship between incarceration and psychiatric disorders, the effects of
incarceration would ceteris paribus be weaker for conditions with earlier onsets, but this pattern
was not observed in our models. Anxiety disorders, for example, have an especially early onset,
31

but their relationship with incarceration is no more sensitive than that relationship between
incarceration and other conditions. We also do not observe the age at which incarceration first
occurs, except that it occurred in adulthood. Because criminal behavior declines with age,
incarceration likely precedes most of the 12-month disorders we observe, but it will be important
in future research to systematically discern the life-course sensitivity of the psychological effects
of incarceration.
Measurement error is also possible with respect to the diagnoses themselves: it is possible
that former inmates manifest psychiatric disorders in a fashion that does not cohere well with
conventional diagnostic criteria. Something of the sort may be apparent in the unusually strong
relationship between incarceration and dysthymia, especially relative to the relationship between
incarceration and major depression. If prisonization involves the suppression of emotions that
might convey weakness or vulnerability, former inmates might express their distress more
through dysthymia, a milder form of sadness, rather than major depression. In this way,
differences in the effects of incarceration could reflect meaningful taxometric asymmetries, but, if
so, the causal effect of incarceration is diminished by a reporting bias, rendering our results
conservative more than inaccurate. Furthermore, even if this sort of reporting differential is
occurring, there is still a relationship between incarceration and major depression that is
sufficiently strong to withstand a great many control variables.
Our results are conservative from other perspectives as well. If incarceration is the end of
a long trail of contact with the criminal justice system, our controls for childhood background,
age-of-onset, and early onset substance disorders may eliminate the stigmatizing effects of
32

contact with the criminal justice system. This is especially the case in instances where adult
inmates are also incarcerated as juveniles and juvenile incarceration itself exerts some influence
over adult psychiatric disorders. By the same token, incarceration history is based on selfreports. If individuals suppress their histories for fear of discrimination, some fraction of those
with a history of incarceration will be incorrectly coded as not having a history, biasing the
effects of incarceration downward.
A final limitation pertains to the meaning of incarceration itself. Apart from the
measurement of incarceration and how adequately we have measured exposure, we are not able
to explain the effects of incarceration and what incarceration actually reveals remains uncertain.
Incarceration is the outcome of the commission of a crime (or, in short stays, the suspicion of the
commission of one), but it reflects many other things besides, including, in most instances, the
mark of a criminal record; exposure to the prison environment; and exposure to the stress of
discrimination and reintegration after release. Although we have documented effects attributable
to having been previously incarcerated and, thus, show some enduring impact, it is unclear
whether these effects represent the lingering effect of the prison environment per se, the effects of
a criminal record, the stress of reintegration, involvement with criminal behavior, involvement
with the criminal justice system, or other sequelae of a criminal record. These many assorted
mechanisms and processes may ultimately stem from prison and, thus, be considered effects of
incarceration, but they imply different points of intervention and, therefore, deserve fine-grained
attention. The data requirements of pursuing a more fine-grained process are, of course, very
high, but we hope our study points to the value in doing so.
33

Conclusion
In closing it is worth noting that our emphasis on psychiatric disorders as an outcome of
incarceration and a mechanism for explaining the poor reintegration has an ironic edge. A long
tradition in sociological research casts psychiatric disorders as a means of labeling and, thereby,
controlling deviant behavior—thus the well-known alignment of “madness” with “badness”
(Conrad 1992). Although psychiatric labels provide a convenient nomenclature for certain forms
of deviance, our results suggest that treating psychiatric disorders provides a potential strategy
for reintegrating and, thereby, a strategy for normalizing former inmates (see also Pogorzelski,
Wolff, Pan, and Blitz 2005). We encourage the development of new frameworks for
understanding incarceration’s impact and consequences. These frameworks should be sensitive
to the different forces behind selection and those behind causation, as well as the distinction
between what is legally criminalized and what is enduringly consequential.

34

References
Abram, Karen M. and Linda A. Teplin. 1991. "Co-occurring disorders among mentally ill jail
detainees: Implications for public policy." American Psychologist 46:1036-1045.
Adams, Kenneth. 1992. "Adjusting to Prison Life." Crime and Justice: A Review of Research
16:275-359.
Alegria, Margarita, James S. Jackson, Ronald C. Kessler, and David Takeuchi. 2008.
Collaborative Psychiatric Epidemiology Surveys (CPES). Institute for Social Research. Ann
Arbor, MI: Interuniversity Consortium for Political and Social Research.
Allison, Paul D. 2001. Missing Data. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Aneshensel, Carol S., Carolyn M. Rutter, and Peter A. Lachenbruch. 1991. "Social Structure,
Stress, and Mental Health: Competing Conceptual and Analytic Models." American
Sociological Review 56:166-178.
Bukstel, Lee H. and Peter R. Kilmann. 1980. "Psychological Effects of Imprisonment on Confined
Individuals." Psychological Bulletin 88:469-493.
Conrad, Peter. 1992. "Medicalization and Social Control." Annual Review of Sociology 18:209-232.
Courtney, Mark E., Irving Piliavin, Andrew Grogan-Kaylor, and Ande Nesmith. 1998. "Foster
Youth Transitions to Adulthood: A Longitudinal View of Youth Leaving Care." Child
Welfare 80:685-717.
Ditton, Paula M. 1999. Mental Health and Treatment of Inmates and Probationers. Washington,
DC: US Department of Justice, Office of Justice Programs.
Ellickson, Phyllis L., Joan S. Tucker, and David J. Klein. 2003. "Ten-Year Prospective Study of
Public Health Problems Associated With Early Drinking." Pediatrics 111:949-955.
Endicott, J., N. Andreasen, and R.L. Spitzer. 1978. Family History Research Diagnostic Criteria.
New York: Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute.

35

Fazel, Seena and John Danesh. 2002. "Serious Mental Disorder in 23,000 Prisoners: A Systematic
Review of 62 Surveys." Lancet 359:545-550.
Felson, Marcus. 1994. Crime and Everyday Life: Insights and Implications for Society. Newbury
Park, CA: Pine Forge Press.
Freudenberg, Nicholas, Jessie Daniels, Martha Crum, Tiffany Perkins, and Beth E. Richie. 2008.
"Coming Home From Jail: The Social and Health Consequences of Community Reentry
for Women, Male Adolescents, and Their Families and Communities." American Journal
of Public Health 98:S191-202.
Goffman, Erving. 1961. Asylums: Essays on the Social Situation of Mental Patients and Other
Inmates. Garden City, NY: Anchor Books.
Golembeski, Cynthia and Robert Fullilove. 2005. "Criminal (In)Justice in the City and Its
Associated Health Consequences." American Journal of Public Health 95:1701-1706.
Gottfredson, Michael R. and Travis Hirschi. 1990. A General Theory of Crime. Stanford, CA:
Stanford University.
Green, Jennifer Greif, Katie A. McLaughlin, Patricia A. Berglund, Michael J. Gruber, Nancy A.
Sampson, Alan M. Zaslavsky, and Ronald C. Kessler. 2010. "Childhood Adversities and
Adult Psychiatric Disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication I:
Associations With First Onset of DSM-IV Disorders." Archives of General Psychiatry
67:113-123.
Haney, Craig. 2003. "The Psychological Impact of Incarceration: Implications for Postprison
Adjustment." Pp. 33-66 in Prisoners Once Removed: The Impact of Incarceration and
Reentry on Children, Families, and Communities, edited by J. Travis and M. Waul.
Washington, D.C.: The Urban Institute.
—. 2006. Reforming Punishment: Psychological Limits to the Pains of Imprisonment. Washington,
DC: American Psychological Association.
Kendler, KS, JL Silberg, MC Neale, RC Kessler, AC Heath, and LJ Eaves. 1991. "The family
history method: whose psychiatric history is measured?" Am J Psychiatry 148:1501-1504.
36

Kessler, R. C., C. G. Davis, and K. S. Kendler. 1997. "Childhood adversity and adult psychiatric
disorder in the US National Comorbidity Survey." Psychological Medicine 27:1101-1119.
Kessler, Ronald C., Patricia Berglund, Wai Tat Chiu, Olga Demler, Steven Heeringa, Eva Hiripi,
Robert Jin, Beth-Ellen Pennell, Ellen E. Walters, Alan Zaslavsky, and Hui Zheung. 2006.
"The US National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R): design and field
procedures." International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research 13:69-92.
Kessler, Ronald C., Patricia Berglund, Olga Demler, Robert Jin, Kathleen R. Merikangas, and
Ellen E. Walters. 2005. "Lifetime Prevalence and Age-of-Onset Distributions of DSM-IV
Disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication." Archives of General
Psychiatry 62:593-602.
Kessler, Ronald C., Wai Tat Chiu, Olga Demler, and Ellen E. Walters. 2005. "Prevalence, Severity,
and Comorbidity of 12-Month DSM-IV Disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey
Replication." Arch Gen Psychiatry 62:617-627.
Kessler, Ronald C. and Kathleen R. Merikangas. 2004. "The National Comorbidity Survey
Replication (NCS-R): background and aims." International Journal of Methods in
Psychiatric Research 13:60-68.
Kessler, Ronald C. and T. Beidirhan Ustun. 2004. "The World Mental Health (WMH) Survey
Initiative Version of the World Health Organization (WHO) Composite International
Diagnostic Interview (CIDI)." International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research
13:93-121.
Kessler, Ronald C. and Philip S. Wang. 2008. "The Descriptive Epidemiology of Commonly
Occurring Mental Disorders in the United States." Annual Review of Public Health
29:115-129.
Knäuper, Bärbel, Charles F. Cannell, Norbert Schwarz, Martha L. Bruce, and Ronald C. Kessler.
1999. "Improving accuracy of major depression age-of-onset reports in the US National
Comorbidity Survey." International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research 8:39-48.
Link, Bruce G. and Jo C. Phelan. 2001. "Conceptualizing Stigma." Annual Review of Sociology
27:363-385.

37

Little, Roderick J. A. and Donald B. Rubin. 2002. Statistical Analysis with Missing Data.
Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Mallik-Kane, Kamala and Christy A. Visher. 2008. Health and Prisoner Reentry: How Physical,
Mental, and Substance Abuse Conditions Shape the Process of Reintegration. Washington,
DC: Urban Institute.
Massoglia, Michael. 2008. "Incarceration as Exposure: The Prison, Infectious Disease, and Other
Stress-Related Illnesses." Journal of Health and Social Behavior 16:56-71.
Massoglia, Michael and Jason Schnittker. 2009. "No Real Release." Contexts 8:38-42.
McCarty, Carolyn A., Beth E. Ebel, Michelle M. Garrison, David L. DiGiuseppe, Dimitri A.
Christakis, and Frederick P. Rivara. 2004. "Continuity of Binge and Harmful Drinking
From Late Adolescence to Early Adulthood." Pediatrics 114:714-719.
Merikangas, Kathleen R., Minnie Ames, Lihong Cui, Paul E. Stang, T. Bedirhan Ustun, Michael
Von Korff, and Ronald C. Kessler. 2007. "The Impact of Comorbidity of Mental and
Physical Conditions on Role Disability in the US Adult Household Population." Archives
of General Psychiatry 64:1180-1188.
Moeller, F. Gerard, Ernest S. Barratt, Donald M. Dougherty, Joy M. Schmitz, and Alan C. Swann.
2001. "Psychiatric Aspects of Impulsivity." American Journal of Psychiatry 158:1783-1793.
National Commission on Correctional Health Care. 2002. The Health Status of Soon-To-BeReleased Inmates. Washington, D.C.: National Commission on Correctional Health Care.
Ormel, Johan, Michael VonKorff, T. Bedirhan Ustun, Stefano Pini, Ailsa Korten, and Tineke
Oldehinkel. 1994. "Common Mental Disorders and Disability Across Cultures: Results
From the WHO Collaborative Study on Psychological Problems in General Health Care."
JAMA 272:1741-1748.
Pager, Devah. 2003. "The Mark of a Criminal Record." American Journal of Sociology 108:937975.

38

—. 2008. "Marked: Race, Crime, and Finding Work in an Era of Mass Incarceration." Pp. 683690 in Social Stratification: Class, Race, and Gender in Sociological Perspective, edited by
D. B. Grusky. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
Pager, Devah, Bruce Western, and Bart Bonikowski. 2009. "Discrimination in a Low-Wage Labor
Market: A Field Experiment." American Sociological Review 74:777-799.
Paus, Tomas, Matcheri Keshavan, and Jay N. Giedd. 2008. "Why do many psychiatric disorders
emerge during adolescence?" Nature Reviews Neuroscience 9:947-957.
Pogorzelski, Wendy, Nancy Wolff, Ko-Yu Pan, and Cynthia L. Blitz. 2005. "Behavioral Health
Problems, Ex-Offender Reentry Policies, and the "Second Chance Act"." American Journal
of Public Health 95:1718-1724.
Rehm, Jürgen, T. Bedirhan Üstün, Shekhar Saxena, Christopher B. Nelson, Somnath Chatterji,
Frank Ivis, and Ed Adlaf. 1999. "On the development and psychometric testing of the
WHO screening instrument to assess disablement in the general population."
International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research 8:110-122.
Rhodes, Lorna A. 2000. "Taxonomic Anxieties: Axis I and Axis II in Prison." Medical
Anthropology Quarterly 14:346-373.
—. 2002. "Psychopathy and the Face of Control in Supermax." Ethnography 3:442-466.
Rubin, Donald B. 1987. Multiple Imputation for Nonresponse in Surveys. New York: Wiley.
Sampson, Robert J. and John H. Laub. 1992. "Crime and Deviance in the Life Course." Annual
Review of Sociology 18:63-84.
Schnittker, Jason and Andrea John. 2007. "Enduring Stigma: The Long-Term Effects of
Incarceration on Health." Journal of Health and Social Behavior 16:115-130.
Straus, Murray A. 1979. "Measuring Intrafamily Conflict and Violence: The Conflict Tactics (CT)
Scales." Journal of Marriage and Family 41:75-88.

39

Swartz, James A. and Arthur J. Lurigio. 2007. "Serious Mental Illness and Arrest: The Generalized
Mediating Effect of Substance Use." Crime Delinquency 53:581-604.
Sykes, Gresham M. 1958. The Society of Captives. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University.
Travis, Jeremy. 2005. But They All Come Back: Facing the Challenges of Prisoner Reentry.
Washington, DC: The Urban Institute Press.
U.S. Department of Justice. 2005. Prison and Jail Inmates at Midyear 2004. Washington, DC:
Government Printing Office.
Uggen, Christopher, Jeff Manza, and Melissa Thompson. 2006. "Citizenship, Democracy, and the
Civic Reintegration of Criminal Offenders." The ANNALS of the American Academy of
Political and Social Science 605:281-310.
Uggen, Christopher and Irving Piliavin. 1998. "Asymmetrical Causation and Criminal
Desistance." Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology 88:1399-1422.
Wacquant, Loic. 2009. Punishing the Poor: The Neoliberal Government of Social Insecurity.
Durham, NC: Duke University Press.
Wakefield, Sara and Christopher Uggen. 2010. "Incarceration and Stratification." Annual Review
of Sociology 36:387-406.
Weinberg, S. Kirson. 1942. "Aspects of the Prison's Social Structure." American Journal of
Sociology 47:717-726.
West, Heather C. 2010. Prison Inmates at Midyear 2009--Statistical Tables. Washington, DC: U.S.
Department of Justice, Office of Justice Programs.
West, Heather C. and William J. Sabol. 2009. Prisoners in 2007. Washington, DC: US
Department of Justice, Office of Justice Programs.
Western, Bruce. 2002. "The Impact of Incarceration on Wage Mobility and Inequality." American
Sociological Review 67:526-546.
40

—. 2007. Punishment and Inequality in America. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.
Western, Bruce and Christopher Wildeman. 2009. "The Black Family and Mass Incarceration."
The ANNALS of the American Academy of Political and Social Science 621:221-242.
Wheeler, Stanton. 1961. "Socialization in Correctional Communities." American Sociological
Review 26:697-712.
Wilper, Andrew P., Steffie Woolhandler, J. Wesley Boyd, Karen E. Lasser, Danny McCormick,
David H. Bor, and David U. Himmelstein. 2009. "The Health and Health Care of US
Prisoners: Results of a Nationwide Survey." American Journal of Public Health 99:666672.

41

Table 1. Lifetime and 12-Month Prevalence of Psychiatric Disorders Among Those With and
Without a History of Incarceration: NCS-R (N = 5,692)
Lifetime Prevalence
12-Month Prevalence
No
No
Incarceration Incarceration Incarceration Incarceration
Mean
S.E. Mean
S.E. Mean
S.E. Mean
S.E.
Anxiety Disorders
Panic Disorder
4.4
(.3)
7.4*
(1.0)
2.5
(.2)
4.5*
(.8)
Agoraphobia
2.3
(.2)
3.4
(.7)
1.2
(.1)
2.7*
(.6)
Specific Phobia
12.2
(.5) 16.0*
(1.6)
8.4
(.5) 11.7* (1.3)
Social Phobia
11.3
(.5) 18.6*
(1.5)
6.2
(.3) 11.5* (1.2)
Generalized Anxiety Disorder
7.6
(.4)
9.3
(.9)
3.9
(.2)
5.3
(.8)
Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder
6.3
(.5) 10.8*
(1.5)
3.2
(.2)
6.3* (1.4)
Adult Separation Anxiety
5.7
(.3) 13.2*
(1.6)
1.6
(.2)
4.0*
(.8)
Mood Disorders
Major Depressive Disorder
Dysthymia
Bipolar Disorder
Impulse Control Disorders
Oppositional Defiant Disorder
Conduct Disorder
Attention Deficit Disorder
Intermittent Explosive Disorder

16.1
3.8
3.8

(.6)
(.3)
(.3)

19.8*
5.9*
8.5*

(1.6)
(.8)
(1.0)

6.4
2.0
2.5

(.3)
(.2)
(.2)

9.2*
4.1*
5.8*

(1.0)
(.7)
(.8)

4.5
3.4
3.5
6.7

(.5)
(.4)
(.3)
(.5)

13.7*
18.3*
10.8*
15.7*

(1.4)
(2.3)
(1.2)
(1.6)

.4
.3
1.7
3.4

(.1)
(.1)
(.2)
(.3)

2.4*
2.2*
5.9*
10.3*

(.7)
(.8)
(1.2)
(1.7)

Substance Disorders
Alcohol Abuse
8.4
(.7) 47.0*
(4.8)
1.9
(.2) 10.0* (1.6)
Alcohol Dependence
3.1
(.3) 21.3*
(2.4)
.8
(.2)
5.2* (1.1)
Drug Abuse
4.9
(.5) 29.2*
(3.1)
.8
(.1)
4.8* (1.0)
Drug Dependence
1.7
(.2) 12.9*
(1.7)
.3
(.1)
1.5*
(.6)
* p < .05 (two-tailed test of mean difference between no incarceration and incarceration; standard
errors are presented in parentheses)
Note: Models based on 20 multiple-imputation data sets, imputing 488 missing cases.

Table 2. Descriptive Epidemiology of Psychiatric Disorders Among Those With and Without a History of
Incarceration: NCS-R (Total N = 5,692)
Percent of 12-Month Cases
Average Age of Onset
To Lifetime Cases
No Incarceration
Mean
S.E.

Incarceration
Mean
S.E.

No Incarceration
Mean
S.E.

Incarceration
Mean
S.E.

Anxiety Disorders
Panic Disorder
Agoraphobia
Specific Phobia
Social Phobia
Generalized Anxiety Disorder
Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder
Adult Separation Anxiety

24.1
19.1
8.4
11.7
27.6
21.2
22.5

(.9)
(1.0)
(.3)
(.3)
(.7)
(.7)
(.7)

20.4*
15.9
9.7
11.4
23.6*
20.8
22.2

(1.3)
(1.7)
(.9)
(.5)
(1.2)
(1.6)
(1.1)

57.9
54.4
69.1
54.8
51.1
50.6
28.5

(2.9)
(4.3)
(1.7)
(1.9)
(2.2)
(3.0)
(2.8)

61.5
79.6*
73.3
61.8
57.2
58.7
30.2

(5.8)
(6.9)
(4.4)
(4.2)
(5.3)
(5.3)
(4.5)

Mood Disorders
Major Depressive Disorder
Dysthymia
Bipolar Disorder

27.2
26.4
23.4

(.4)
(.9)
(.8)

24.6*
21.3*
22.7

(1.2)
(1.4)
(1.4)

39.4
54.5
64.2

(1.5)
(3.3)
(3.1)

46.4
68.9
68.2

(4.2)
(6.5)
(5.3)

Impulse Control Disorders
Oppositional Defiant Disorder
Conduct Disorder
Attention Deficit Disorder
Intermittent Explosive Disorder

10.8
11.4
6.6
14.5

(.3)
(.3)
(.2)
(.4)

10.0
11.7
7.2
13.4

(.4)
(.3)
(.3)
(.5)

8.6
9.0
47.9
51.0

(1.7)
(2.6)
(3.7)
(2.6)

17.7
12.0
54.6
65.7*

(4.3)
(3.8)
(6.4)
(4.6)

Substance Disorders
Alcohol Abuse
22.6
(.5)
21.2*
(.4)
22.6
(2.1)
21.4
(2.5)
Alcohol Dependence
23.9
(.6)
22.9
(.6)
25.2
(3.5)
24.1
(3.9)
Drug Abuse
19.3
(.3)
20.0
(.4)
16.7
(2.5)
16.2
(2.5)
Drug Dependence
20.3
(.6)
22.6*
(.9)
15.9
(3.7)
11.6
(3.5)
* p < .05 (two-tailed test of mean difference between no incarceration and incarceration; standard errors are presented
in parentheses)
Note: Models based on 20 multiple-imputation data sets, imputing 488 missing cases.

Table 3. Childhood Background and Incarceration: NCS-R (N=5,692)

Childhood Adversities
Interpersonal Loss [0-1]
Family Maladaptation [0-4]
Economic Adversity [0-1]
Abuse or Neglect [0-3]
Early Onset Substance Abuse
Alcohol Abuse [0-1]
Drug Abuse [0-1]

No Incarceration
Mean
S.E.

Incarceration
Mean
S.E.

.30
.49
.09
.31

(.01)
(.02)
(.01)
(.01)

.42*
.82*
.12*
.48*

(.02)
(.05)
(.02)
(.03)

.02
.02

(.00)
(.00)

.15*
.11*

(.02)
(.01)

* p < .05 (two-tailed test of mean difference between no incarceration and incarceration;
standard errors are presented in parentheses; variable ranges presented in brackets)
Note: Models based on 20 multiple-imputation data sets, imputing 488 missing cases.

Table 4. Any Incarceration Coefficients from Logit Regression Models of Lifetime Disorders with Controls: NCS-R (N=5,692)
Model 1
Model 2
Model 3
Model 4
Model 3 + Eliminating
Model 1 + Childhood
Model 2 + Early-Onset
those with Under 18 Onset
Basic Controls
Background
Substance Abuse
of Primary Disorder
Coef.
S.E.
Coef.
S.E.
Coef.
S.E.
Coef.
S.E.
Anxiety Disorders
Panic Disorder
0.829**
(0.181)
0.574**
(0.176)
0.473**
(0.175)
0.559*
(0.208)
Agoraphobia
0.507
(0.286)
0.210
(0.290)
0.114
(0.272)
0.117
(0.394)
Specific Phobia
0.499**
(0.141)
0.288*
(0.136)
0.209
(0.137)
0.939
(0.483)
Social Phobia
0.651**
(0.121)
0.477**
(0.125)
0.405**
(0.123)
0.533
(0.288)
Generalized Anxiety
0.551**
(0.134)
0.276*
(0.131)
0.235
(0.130)
0.367
(0.200)
Posttraumatic Stress
1.004**
(0.211)
0.714**
(0.208)
0.610**
(0.206)
0.697*
(0.278)
Adult Separation Anxiety 0.917**
(0.163)
0.672**
(0.150)
0.600**
(0.147)
0.797**
(0.164)
Mood Disorders
Major Depressive
Dysthymia
Bipolar

0.500**
0.744**
0.738**

(0.113)
(0.183)
(0.149)

0.322**
0.390*
0.477**

(0.113)
(0.185)
(0.149)

0.288*
0.340
0.321*

(0.112)
(0.186)
(0.137)

0.375*
0.537*
0.511*

(0.142)
(0.240)
(0.194)

Impulse Control Disorders
Intermittent Explosive

0.653**

(0.144)

0.481**

(0.140)

0.371*

(0.138)

0.527

(0.270)

Substance Disorders
Alcohol Abuse
2.029**
(0.218)
1.901**
(0.212)
Alcohol Dependence
1.922**
(0.197)
1.750**
(0.191)
Drug Abuse
1.862**
(0.203)
1.704**
(0.199)
Drug Dependence
2.017**
(0.245)
1.794**
(0.244)
* p < .05; ** p < .01 (two-tailed test; standard errors are in parentheses)
Note: The basic controls are education, race/ethnicity, age, and sex. Models based on 20 multiple-imputation data sets, imputing 488 missing cases.

Table 5. Any Incarceration Coefficients from Logit Regression Models of 12-Month Disorders with Controls: NCS-R (N=5,692)
Model 1
Model 2
Model 3
Model 4
Model 3 + Controls
Model 1 + Childhood
Model 2 + Early-Onset
for Under 18 Onset of
Basic Controls
Background
Substance Abuse
Primary Disorder
Coef.
S.E.
Coef.
S.E.
Coef.
S.E.
Coef.
S.E.
Anxiety Disorders
Panic Disorder
0.855**
(0.232)
0.570*
(0.222)
0.464*
(0.225)
0.509
(0.259)
Agoraphobia
0.731*
(0.297)
0.491
(0.302)
0.414
(0.296)
0.592
(0.366)
Specific Phobia
0.568**
(0.143)
0.366**
(0.135)
0.311*
(0.136)
0.598**
(0.202)
Social Phobia
0.733**
(0.152)
0.539**
(0.147)
0.491**
(0.153)
0.324
(0.202)
Generalized Anxiety
0.568**
(0.174)
0.308
(0.175)
0.255
(0.176)
0.371
(0.240)
Posttraumatic Stress
1.141**
(0.298)
0.772*
(0.297)
0.631*
(0.296)
0.578
(0.380)
Adult Separation Anxiety 0.759**
(0.276)
0.540
(0.286)
0.47
(0.302)
0.504
(0.303)
Mood Disorders
Major Depressive
Dysthymia
Bipolar

0.597**
0.954**
0.701**

(0.148)
(0.239)
(0.169)

0.395**
0.657**
0.480**

(0.135)
(0.212)
(0.170)

0.383**
0.626**
0.335*

(0.141)
(0.226)
(0.166)

0.392*
0.748**
0.484

(0.157)
(0.264)
(0.259)

Impulse Control Disorders
Intermittent Explosive

0.915**

(0.198)

0.737**

(0.197)

0.628**

(0.204)

0.632*

(0.258)

Substance Disorders
Alcohol Abuse
1.468**
(0.231)
1.304**
(0.240)
Alcohol Dependence
1.709**
(0.339)
1.486**
(0.356)
Drug Abuse
1.420**
(0.331)
1.224**
(0.338)
Drug Dependence
1.549**
(0.492)
1.241*
(0.494)
* p < .05; ** p < .01 (two-tailed test; standard errors are in parentheses)
Note: The basic controls are education, race/ethnicity, age, and sex. Models based on 20 multiple-imputation data sets, imputing 488 missing cases.

Table 6. Linear Regression Models of WHO-DAS Disability Scores: NCS-R (N=5,692)

Any incarceration
Education
High School Diploma
13 to 15 Years
16 or More Years

Model 1
0.550*
(0.258)

Self-Care
Model 2
0.427
(0.278)

Model 3
0.240
(0.266)

Model 1
0.903**
(0.277)

Cognitive
Model 2 Model 3
0.566*
0.069
(0.233)
(0.202)

Model 1
3.312**
(0.940)

Mobility
Model 2
2.827**
(0.934)

Model 3
1.921*
(0.893)

-1.310**
(0.482)
-1.298**
(0.452)
-0.987
(0.525)

-1.274*
(0.498)
-1.276**
(0.458)
-0.952
(0.583)

-1.141*
(0.494)
-1.124*
(0.455)
-0.713
(0.557)

-0.430*
(0.207)
-0.316
(0.239)
-0.679**
(0.242)

-0.370
(0.194)
-0.267
(0.231)
-0.565*
(0.239)

-1.900
(1.212)
-2.407*
(1.000)
-3.384**
(1.065)

-1.771
(1.206)
-2.322*
(1.011)
-3.261**
(1.085)

-1.036
(1.302)
-1.472
(1.071)
-1.892
(1.154)

Condition Counts
Anxiety

0.441*
(0.189)
0.845*
(0.375)
-0.438
(0.400)
-0.014
(0.211)
0.510**
(0.159)

Mood
Impulse
Substance
Chronic Physical
Conditions

Any incarceration
Education
High School Diploma
13 to 15 Years
16 or More Years
Condition Counts
Anxiety

-0.095
(0.194)
0.029
(0.194)
-0.104
(0.202)
1.365**
(0.189)
2.312**
(0.357)
0.276
(0.329)
-0.167
(0.260)
0.640**
(0.073)

1.393**
(0.405)
2.650**
(0.738)
-0.568
(0.701)
-0.956*
(0.431)
3.126**
(0.225)

Extent Out of Role
Model 1 Model 2 Model 3
8.499**
6.965**
4.262**
(1.698)
(1.628)
(1.355)

Social Interaction
Model 1 Model 2 Model 3
1.004**
0.801**
0.400
(0.282)
(0.264)
(0.209)

Social Participation
Model 1 Model 2 Model 3
2.554**
1.918**
0.997*
(0.642)
(0.588)
(0.446)

-3.104
(2.034)
-4.831**
(1.609)
-6.787**
(1.596)

-0.027
(0.196)
-0.054
(0.207)
-0.067
(0.218)

-0.729*
(0.349)
-0.778
(0.397)
-1.261**
(0.363)

-2.489
(2.008)
-4.188*
(1.553)
-5.746**
(1.557)

-0.919
(2.074)
-2.380
(1.535)
-2.897
(1.643)

0.028
(0.198)
-0.005
(0.203)
0.023
(0.211)

0.213
(0.197)
0.192
(0.179)
0.323
(0.187)

-0.578
(0.339)
-0.661
(0.400)
-1.035**
(0.372)

-0.093
(0.327)
-0.127
(0.339)
-0.204
(0.314)

4.152**
1.064**
2.018**
(0.501)
(0.183)
(0.311)
Mood
7.784**
1.952**
3.819**
(1.026)
(0.338)
(0.520)
Impulse
1.539
0.376
0.760
(1.384)
(0.335)
(0.633)
Substance
1.598
0.147
0.234
(1.144)
(0.231)
(0.958)
Chronic Physical
5.321**
0.286**
1.201**
Conditions
(0.424)
(0.063)
(0.123)
* p < .05; ** p < .01 (two-tailed test; standard errors are in parentheses)
Note: All models also include covariates for race/ethnicity, age, and sex. Models based on 20 multiple-imputation data sets,
imputing 488 missing cases.

Figure 1. Conceptual Model Illustrating Influences in the Incarceration-Psychiatric Disorder Relationship

Early-Onset
Psychiatric Disorders

Incarceration

Childhood
Adversities

Early-Onset
Substance Disorders

Psychiatric Disorders

Disability in Social
and Economic Roles